(You, Your Brain, and the Nature vs. Nurture Debate)
Imagine your life is a tape, and we rewind it. All your accomplishments, awards, and graduations are erased. Your experiences move in reverse, and you grow smaller and smaller, as you were as a child. Your adult teeth turn back into baby teeth, and eventually retract completely, while all of your traits and quirks start to fade away. Pretty soon language goes away too, and you’re no longer you, but potential you. The tape continues rewinding, halving colonies of your cells until finally we arrive at the amazing singular miracle: the one cell that will evolve to become you.
Now, the question is this: what happens when we press “play”
again? The common battle between nature versus nurture arises – the question
that psychologists and scientists everywhere are prodding, probing, and
researching to figure out. Are your talents, traits, and personal
characteristics deeply embedded in your genes? Is what makes you you implanted in the DNA of our cells?
Or could things completely change who we are with a few simple nudges? To put
it simply – how much of your fate do you believe depends on your genes, your
surroundings, or merely just by chance?
Since we can’t rewind time, Julia Freund and her colleagues
found another way to better answer this question in a simple but remarkable recent
study. To test the nature-versus-nurture phenomenon, Freund and her
investigators placed genetically identical mice in a common environment, and
tested to see whether systematic behavioral differences still could emerge. By
answering “yes,” it would mean that there are sources of behavioral variability
(intrinsic individuality, if you will) that could be unaccounted for by a
combination of common genes and a common environment.
For the experiment, 40 genetically identical mice were
placed in an “enriched” environment, where Freund monitored their behavior for
a period of three months (which, for mice, is about 10-15% of their entire
lifespan). The enriched environment was about 36 square feet, engineered to
include multi-tiered platforms, nesting boxes, and interconnected tubes in
order to promote exploratory behaviors in the mice that would not exist in a
normal confined cage. What makes this study different from one of human twins
is that by using mice, the subjects’ movements could be recorded with
extraordinary detail over a major period of their lifespan. A radiofrequency ID
transponder was placed on every mouse, measuring their every movement, chase,
and sedentary period.
In order to measure the differences in behavior between the
mice, the investigators used a gage called “roaming entropy.” Roaming entropy
captures how often you get out, and with how much variety – so basically if you
are someone who just darts back and forth (say from your dorm room to Van
Pelt…) your roaming entropy is low. But if you’re the type of person who could
pretty much be anywhere at any given time, you have a high roaming entropy. At
the beginning of the study, the mice all had fairly similar roaming entropies…
however, as the weeks progressed, the population diverged significantly, with
some mice being much more exploratory and active than others. If you take the
tendency to explore as a characteristic trait, then this is obviously one that
elaborates and changes over time in a way that isn’t strictly determined by genes
or the environment.
However, the most interesting part of the study arose when
Fruend and her team examined the changes in brain activity that went along with
the changes in exploratory behavior. Before the experiment was over, the mice
were injected with a compound that selectively incorporates itself into
dividing cells. This basically means that the compound can show researchers
which neurons are formed in adulthood, and which neurons the mice were born
with. While most of our neurons are formed during early development, there are
a good number of well-studied brain areas that continuously produce new neurons
throughout our adult lives.
Surprisingly, the mice that were the most exploratory
throughout the study (who exhibited the most outgoing behavior) were also those
who experienced the greatest production of adult-born neurons. While we can’t
say this particularly proves
anything, the results are still pretty intriguing. Even after your genes are
set in stone from birth, and the majority of your environmental surroundings
are laid firm throughout your early development, your brain maintains the raw
potential to grow its own new neurons. The investigators of this study propose
that these neurons are involved in tailoring and tuning our behaviors, implying
that the way we live our lives may
make us who we are.
So, how does this happen? We don’t actually know. No
disrespect intended to these researchers, but any experiment addressing such a
controversial, profound, and metaphysically-tangled problem as the
nature-vs.-nurture debate is going to generate more questions than answers. It
could be that epigenetic changes, where experience
modifies gene expression, gives rise
to completely different life paths. It can also be questioned just how
substantial the differences in roaming entropy could actually be, and whether
it was actually statistically significant. Regardless of the specifics and
questions left to be answered, this experiment is a reminder that our lives are
truly a work in progress. Whether it is our genes, our environments, or
generation of adult born neurons, the nature versus nurture debate is yet to be
solved. But it seems that if we are
living out our lives as a sort of tape, then it’s a tape in which the tracks
can be tweaked as they’re read, as our genes can be modified as we live. As
your brain is shaped by your life and vice versa, there is so much room for
chance and noise – room for you to become you.